研究文章
资源中心收集了大量的文章供读者参考
教务处组织的数字教材建设与应用实践专题讲座,为教师们提供了宝贵的学习和交流机会,推动了数字教材的发展与创新。
教务处组织的数字教材建设与应用实践专题讲座,为教师们提供了宝贵的学习和交流机会,推动了数字教材的发展与创新。
Siller, M., Morgan, L., Fuhrmeister, S., Wedderburn, Q., Schirmer, B., Chatson, E., & Gillespie, S. (2024). Feasibility and acceptability of a low-resource-intensive, transdiagnostic intervention for children with social-communication challenges in early childhood education settings. Autism,28(3), 587-599.
Abstract
Preschool classrooms provide a unique context for supporting the development of children with social-communication challenges. This study is an uncontrolled clinical trial of an adapted professional development intervention for preschool teachers (Social Emotional Engagement-Knowledge & Skills-Early Childhood). Social Emotional Engagement-Knowledge & Skills-Early Childhood is a low-resource-intensive, transdiagnostic intervention to address the learning needs of children with social-communication challenges and consists of four asynchronous online modules and three synchronous coaching sessions. The current research evaluated the feasibility and acceptability of intervention and research procedures, implemented in authentic early childhood education settings. Participants included one teacher and one target child with social-communication challenges from 25 preschool classrooms, sampled to maximize variability. Overall, the current research revealed high levels of feasibility, with 9 out of 10 benchmarks met: (a) procedures for participant recruitment reliably identified a neurodiverse sample of children with teacher-reported social-communication challenges; (b) teachers showed high levels of program engagement and Social Emotional Engagement-Knowledge & Skills-Early Childhood completion (76%); and (c) results revealed a robust pattern of gains in Social Emotional Engagement-Knowledge & Skills-Early Childhood classrooms and associations among key outcome measures (including active engagement, student teacher relationship, social-communication competencies). Implications for the design of a subsequent, larger effectiveness-implementation hybrid trial (Type 1) are discussed.
学前教室为支持有社交挑战的儿童的发展提供了一个独特的环境。本研究是一项非对照临床试验,聚焦于学前教师的适应性专业发展干预(社会情绪参与知识和童年早期技能)。社会情绪参与知识和童年早期技能是一种低资源密集型、经诊断的干预措施,用以应对有社交挑战的儿童的学习需求,包括四个异步在线模块和三个同步辅导课程。本研究评估了在真实的早期教育环境中,干预和研究程序的可行性和可接受性。参与者包括来自25对学前班的教师-社交挑战儿童组,采样以最大限度地提高多样性。总体上,本研究结果显示出较高的可行性,10个基准中有9个达到标准:(a)招募参与者的程序通过教师报告社交调挑战,可靠地确定了一个神经多样化儿童样本;(b)教师表现出高水平的参与与实施(76%);(c)结果显示了,社会情绪参与知识和童年早期技能教室和协会在关键指标结果上((包括积极参与、学生教师关系、社会沟通能力)),均显示出强劲的增长。本研究还讨论了后续的一个更大范围有效实施的混合试验(类型1)设计。
Zhang, S., Li, H., Li, H., & Zhao, S. (2024). The effect of autistic traits on prosocial behavior: The chain mediating role of received social support and perceived social support. Autism, 28(3), 600-615.
Abstract
There is growing evidence that the defining characteristics of autism spectrum disorder are distributed across the general population; therefore, understanding the correlates of prosocial behavior in individuals with high levels of autistic traits could shed light on autism spectrum disorder and prosocial behavior. In this study, the mechanism underlying the influence of individuals’ autistic traits on their prosocial behavior was explored by conducting a questionnaire survey of 414 Chinese college students. The results showed that autistic traits can influence individuals’ prosocial behavior not only through the separate effects of received social support and perceived social support but also through the chain mediating effects of received social support and perceived social support; however, the direct effect of autistic traits on individuals’ prosocial behavior is not significant. This study is conducive to understanding the internal mechanism underlying the relationship between autistic traits and prosocial behavior. Future work is required to further investigate the clinical autism spectrum disorder samples and cross-cultural applicability of the model found in this study.
越来越多的证据表明,孤独症谱系障碍人群的特质在一般人群亦有分布,因此,了解高功能孤独症人群特质与亲社会行为间的关系,有助于明晰孤独症与亲社会行为。本研究通过对414名中国大学生进行问卷调查,探讨了个体的孤独症特征对其亲社会行为的影响机制。结果表明,孤独症特征影响个体亲社会行为的途径,不仅是接受性社会支持和领悟性社会支持的单独效应,并且还有二者间的链式中介效应。然而,孤独症特质对个体亲社会行为的直接影响并不显著。本研究有助于理解孤独症特质与亲社会行为之间关系的内在机制。未来的工作需要进一步研究孤独症临床样本和本模型的跨文化应用。
作者:Larissa L. Faustmann,Mareike Altgassen
摘要 The ability to create mental representations of scenes is essential for remembering, predicting, and imagining. In individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) this ability may be impaired. Considering that autistic characteristics such as weak central coherence or reduced communication abilities may disadvantage autistic
participants in traditional imagery tasks, this study attempted to use a novel task design to measure the ability of scene imagery. Thirty high-functioning adults with ASD and 27 non-autistic matched control adults were asked to describe imagined fictitious scenes using two types of scene imagery tasks. In a free imagery task, participants were asked to imagine a scene based on a given keyword. In a guided imagery task, participants had to imagine a scene based on a detailed description of the scene. Additionally, narrative abilities were assessed using the Narrative Scoring Scheme. Statistical analyses revealed no group effects in the free and guided imagery of fictional scenes. Participants with ASD performed worse than control participants in the narrative task. Narrative abilities correlated positively with performance in both imagery tasks in the ASD group only. Hence, individuals with ASD seem to show as good imagery abilities as non-autistic individuals. The results are discussed in the light of the differences between imagery and imagination and possible gender differences.
Lay Summary
The ability to imagine is an essential basis for a wide range of cognitive functions such as thinking about the future, remembering past information, or fantasizing about (im)possible events. Previous studies suggest impairments in imagery abilities in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. However, we hypothesize that the traditional task designs used in past studies may have disadvantaged individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) given the tasks’ high demands on fantasy, verbal ability, and central coherence which are often compromised in ASD.Using a novel approach—taking the characteristic neural processing style in ASD into account—, we aim to re-examine imagery in this population.
摘要 创造场景的心理表征的能力对于记忆、预测和想象是必不可少的。在患有自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)的人中,这种能力可能会受到损害。考虑到自闭症患者的特点,如中枢连贯性较弱或沟通能力降低,在传统表象任务中可能会对自闭症参与者不利,本研究尝试使用一种新颖的任务设计来测量场景表象能力。30名患有自闭症的高功能成年人和27名非自闭症对照成年人被要求使用两种类型的场景想象任务描述想象的虚构场景。在一项自由成像任务中,参与者被要求根据给定的关键字想象一个场景。在引导式成像任务中,参与者必须根据场景的详细描述来想象场景。此外,还使用叙事评分方案评估了叙事能力。统计分析表明,在虚构场景的自由和引导意象中,没有群体效应。患有自闭症的参与者在叙事任务中的表现比对照参与者差。只有自闭症组的叙事能力与两种表象任务的表现呈正相关。因此,自闭症患者的表象能力似乎和非自闭症患者一样好。根据表象和想象之间的差异以及可能的性别差异对结果进行了讨论。
想象能力是一系列认知功能的重要基础,如思考未来、记住过去的信息或幻想可能发生的事件。先前的研究表明,自闭症谱系障碍患者的表象能力存在损害。然而,我们假设,过去研究中使用的传统任务设计可能对自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)患者不利,因为这些任务对幻想、言语能力和中央一致性的要求很高,而ASD往往会损害这些要求。使用一种新的方法–考虑到ASD特有的神经处理方式–我们的目标是重新检查这一人群中的图像。
Nathan C. Foster1,Simon J. Bennett, Kiri Pullar1,Joe Causer et al.
Observing and voluntarily imitating the biological kinematics displayed by a
model underpins the acquisition of new motor skills via sensorimotor processes linking perception with action. Differences in voluntary imitation in autism could be related to sensorimotor processing activity during action-observation of biological motion, as well as how sensorimotor integration processing occurs across imitation attempts. Using an observational practice protocol, which minimized the active contribution of the peripheral sensorimotor system, we examined the contribution of sensorimotor processing during action-observation. The data showed that autistic participants imitated both the temporal duration and atypical kinematic profile of the observed movement with a similar level of accuracy as neurotypical participants. These findings suggest the lower-level perception-action processes responsible for encoding biological kinematics during the action observation phase of imitation are operational in autism. As there was no task-specific engagement of the peripheral sensorimotor system during observational practice, imitation difficulties in autism are most likely underpinned by sensorimotor integration issues related to the processing of efferent and (re)afferent sen-
sorimotor information during trial-to-trial motor execution.
Lay Summary
Learning a skill by imitating a model (i.e., teacher, parent, or carer) occurs in many everyday situations such as a classroom or home. Imitation can be difficult for some autistic people, especially if a skill is new or complex. These difficulties are said to be based on how autistic people watch a skill and subsequently process what they watched in order to imitate the skill. In this study, we tasked autistic people to learn a new skill by only watching (not imitating) a model during a period of practice. Autistic people imitated how the model moved (movement time and style) similarly to non-autistic people. This finding is very important because it showed that the reported difficulty in imitation is not based on how autistic people watch and process a model (i.e., how they move), but a result of sensory-motor difficulties related to how they plan and get ready to imitate a model. Parents/carers, clinicians, teachers, and/or support workers should therefore consider autistic motor difficulties when teaching new motor skills and everyday skills via modeling.
摘要 观察和自愿地模仿模型所显示的生物运动学是通过将感知与行动联系起来的感觉运动过程来获得新的运动技能的基础。自闭症患者自愿模仿的差异可能与动作-观察生物运动过程中的感觉运动加工活动有关,以及感觉运动整合加工是如何在模仿尝试中发生的。使用观察练习方案,将外周感觉运动系统的积极贡献降至最低,我们检查了感觉运动处理在动作观察中的贡献。数据显示,自闭症参与者模仿观察到的运动的时间持续时间和非典型运动学特征,其准确度与神经典型参与者相似。这些发现表明,在模仿的动作观察阶段,负责编码生物运动学的较低水平的知觉-行动过程在自闭症中是可操作的。由于在观察实践中没有外周感觉运动系统的特定任务参与,自闭症的模仿困难很可能是由与试验到试验运动执行过程中传出和(重新)传入感觉运动信息的处理有关的感觉运动整合问题所支撑的。
通过模仿模型(例如,老师、家长或照顾者)学习一项技能在许多日常情况下都会发生,例如在教室或家里。对于一些自闭症患者来说,模仿可能很困难,特别是当一项技能是新的或复杂的时候。据说,这些困难是基于自闭症患者观看一项技能,然后为了模仿这项技能而对所观看的内容进行加工。在这项研究中,我们让自闭症患者在一段时间的练习中只看(而不是模仿)一个模型来学习一项新技能。自闭症患者模仿模型的移动方式(动作、时间和风格),与非自闭症患者相似。这一发现非常重要,因为它表明,人们报告的模仿困难并不是基于自闭症患者观看和处理模型的方式(即他们如何移动),而是与他们如何计划和准备模仿模型有关的感觉-运动困难的结果。因此,父母/护理人员、临床医生、教师和/或支持人员在通过建模教授新的运动技能和日常技能时,应该考虑自闭症运动障碍。
Hillary Schiltz , Dena Gohari, Jamie Park and Catherine Lord.
Many autistic people and people with non-spectrum neurodevelopmental disabilities (e.g. intellectual disability) report feeling lonely, which can negatively impact their well-being. There is little longitudinal research, however, tracking changes
in how autistic people experience, conceptualize, and cope with loneliness throughout their lives. A longitudinal sample of 114 people, which included autistic participants and participants with neurodevelopmental disabilities, characterized experiences of loneliness, perceptions of other people’s loneliness, and strategies used to cope with loneliness from childhood to adulthood. Level of loneliness and coping strategies were coded from Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule Modules 3 and 4 protocol forms. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule Loneliness Ratings were correlated across time and increased from adolescence to young adulthood. The most common loneliness coping strategies were Behavioral Distraction (e.g. watching TV) and Instrumental Action (e.g. seeking social contact), which were both used by more people in adulthood than childhood. Those who used Behavioral Distraction and a greater number of coping strategies had higher Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule Loneliness–Self Ratings (i.e. were lonelier) during adolescence and adulthood. Findings highlight adulthood as a particularly vulnerable time for loneliness and indicate a need for more support and social opportunities for autistic adults and adults with neurodevelopmental disabilities who wish to make more social connections.
Lay abstract
We know that many autistic people feel lonely, but we don’t know whether their loneliness changes over time. Our research study followed autistic people and people with other non-spectrum neurodevelopmental disabilities from childhood through young adulthood and asked them about their loneliness. While many people told us they felt lonely or very lonely, a sizable group also told us that they do not feel lonely. We found that people who reported feeling lonely earlier in life were likely to also report feeling lonely later in life. Overall, autistic people and people with other neurodevelopmental disabilities in our study became lonelier from adolescence to adulthood. People described multiple ways they cope with feeling lonely, such as distracting themselves or reaching out to connect with another person. People who used distraction tended to be lonelier than those who did not. Our findings tell us that there is a need for greater support of social connections for many autistic people as they become adults.
摘要 许多自闭症患者和患有非谱系神经发育障碍(如智力障碍)的人报告说感到孤独,这可能会对他们的幸福感产生负面影响。然而,很少有纵向研究跟踪自闭症患者在一生中如何体验、概念化和应对孤独的变化。一项114人的纵向样本,包括自闭症参与者和有神经发育障碍的参与者,描述了孤独的经历,对其他人孤独的感知,以及从童年到成年应对孤独的策略。孤独感水平和应对策略由自闭症诊断观察量表3和4协议表编码。孤独症诊断观察表孤独感评分随时间变化呈正相关,且从青春期到成年期呈递增趋势。最常见的孤独应对策略是行为分心(例如看电视)和工具性行动(例如寻求社交),这两种策略在成年后使用的人数都多于儿童时期。在青春期和成年期,使用行为分心和较多应对策略的自闭症诊断观察表孤独自评(即更孤独)较高。研究结果突出表明,成年是孤独的特别脆弱的时期,并表明希望建立更多社会关系的自闭症成年人和患有神经发育障碍的成年人需要更多的支持和社交机会。
我们知道许多自闭症患者感到孤独,但我们不知道他们的孤独是否会随着时间的推移而改变。我们的研究跟踪了自闭症患者和患有其他非谱系神经发育障碍的人,从童年到成年,询问他们的孤独感。虽然许多人告诉我们他们感到孤独或非常孤独,但也有相当一部分人告诉我们他们不感到孤独。我们发现,那些在生活早期感到孤独的人很可能在以后的生活中也会感到孤独。总体而言,在我们的研究中,自闭症患者和其他神经发育障碍患者从青春期到成年期变得更加孤独。人们描述了多种应对孤独感的方法,比如分散自己的注意力,或者伸手与另一个人联系。使用分心技术的人往往比不使用分心技术的人更孤独。我们的发现告诉我们,随着许多自闭症患者成年,他们需要更多的社会关系支持。
Stephen J Gentles, Elise C Ng-Cordell, Michelle C Hunsche,et al.
Researchers increasingly employ longitudinal trajectory methods to understand developmental pathways of people on the autism spectrum across the lifespan. By assessing developmental or health-related outcome domains at three or more timepoints, trajectory studies can characterize their shape and varying rates of change over time. The purpose of this scoping review was to identify and summarize the published breadth of research that uses a trajectory study design to examine development in children (to age 18 years) diagnosed with autism. Using a systematic search and
screening procedure, 103 studies were included. This review summarizes methodological characteristics across studies including the varying statistical approaches used. A series of figures maps where published research is available across
10 outcome domains and the ages over which children have been followed. Evidence gaps, informed by the perspectives of the autistic and caregiver stakeholders that were engaged in this review, are discussed. We recommend that future trajectory research addresses the absence of studies from low- and middle-income countries, considers longitudinal assessment of outcome domains that caregivers and autistic people consider meaningful, and plans follow-up periods with assessment timepoints that cover the gaps in ages where more outcome-specific data are needed.
Lay Abstract
The types of outcomes studied in children on the autism spectrum include clinical characteristics, such as social functioning, communication, language, or autism symptoms. Research that measures these outcomes at multiple timepoints is useful
to improve our understanding of what to expect as children develop. In trajectory studies, researchers assess outcomes at three or more timepoints. This method has advantages over two-timepoint studies because it allows researchers to describe changes in the speed of development, such as accelerations, plateaus, or slowdowns. We identified and reviewed 103 published trajectory studies in children (to age 18 years) with an autism diagnosis. Importantly, we did not include studies of treatments or their effects, nor did we summarize the results of studies. Instead, this review summarizes the characteristics of the available published research, including the methods used, the many different outcomes that have been studied over time and the ages over which they have been studied. This summary may be of interest to autistic people and caregivers (parents) who want to know about the existence of research that provides answers about what to expect during an autistic child’s development. We have recommended that future trajectory research efforts try to make up for the lack of studies from low- and middle-income countries; that more attention is given to the following outcomes that are meaningful to caregivers and autistic people; and to try to fill in the age gaps where more outcome-specific data are needed.
摘要 研究人员越来越多地使用纵向轨迹方法来了解自闭症患者一生中的发展路径。通过评估三个或三个以上时间点的发育或健康相关结果领域,轨迹研究可以表征它们的形状和随时间变化的变化速度。这一范围审查的目的是确定和总结已发表的使用轨迹研究设计来检查被诊断为自闭症的儿童(至18岁)的发展的研究广度。使用系统的搜索和筛选程序,纳入103项研究。这篇综述总结了不同研究的方法学特点,包括使用的不同统计方法。一系列图表描绘了在10个结果领域发表的研究成果以及儿童被跟踪的年龄。证据差距,由自闭症患者和照顾者利益相关者的观点,参与了这一审查,进行了讨论。我们建议,未来的轨迹研究应解决低收入和中等收入国家缺乏研究的问题,考虑对照顾者和自闭症患者认为有意义的结果领域进行纵向评估,并计划具有评估时间点的后续阶段,以弥补需要更多特定结果数据的年龄差距。
自闭症谱系中研究的儿童结果的类型包括临床特征,如社交功能、沟通、语言或自闭症症状。在多个时间点衡量这些结果的研究有助于我们更好地理解儿童发育过程中会发生什么。在轨迹研究中,研究人员在三个或更多的时间点评估结果。这种方法比两个时间点的研究更有优势,因为它允许研究人员描述开发速度的变化,如加速、停滞或减速。我们确定并回顾了103项已发表的关于自闭症儿童(至18岁)的轨迹研究。重要的是,我们没有包括对治疗或其影响的研究,也没有总结研究的结果。相反,这篇综述总结了现有已发表研究的特点,包括所使用的方法,随着时间的推移已经研究的许多不同的结果,以及它们被研究的年代。这一总结可能会引起自闭症患者和照顾者(父母)的兴趣,他们想知道是否有研究为自闭症儿童的发展提供了答案。我们建议,未来的轨迹研究努力努力弥补低收入和中等收入国家研究的不足;更多地关注以下对照顾者和自闭症患者有意义的结果;并努力填补需要更多具体结果数据的年龄差距。
浙江省杭州市萧山区耕文路1108号
0571-86933421
info@nsercasd.cn
周一至周五: 8:00-19:00
周六: 8:00-14:00
周日: 休息